When a bird sings, you may think you’re hearing music. But
are the melodies it’s making really music? Or is what we’re hearing merely a
string of lilting calls that appeals to the human ear?
اضافة اعلان
Birdsong has inspired musicians from Bob Marley to Mozart
and perhaps as far back as the first hunter-gatherers who banged out a beat.
And a growing body of research is showing that the affinity human musicians
feel toward birdsong has a strong scientific basis. Scientists are
understanding more about avian species’ ability to learn, interpret and produce
songs much like our own.
Just like humans, birds learn songs from one another and
practice to perfect them. And just as human speech is distinct from human
music, bird calls, which serve as warnings and other forms of direct
communication, differ from birdsong.
While researchers are still debating the functions of
birdsong, studies show that it is structurally similar to our own tunes. So,
are birds making music? That depends on what you mean.
“I’m not sure we can or want to define music,” said Ofer
Tchernichovski, a zoologist and psychologist at the City University of New York
who studies birdsong.
Where you draw the line between music and mere noise is
arbitrary, said Emily Doolittle, a zoomusicologist and composer at the Royal
Conservatoire of Scotland. The difference between a human baby’s babbling
versus a toddler’s humming might seem more distinct than that of a hatchling’s
cry for food and a maturing bird’s practicing of a melody, she added.
Wherever we draw the line, birdsong, and human song share
striking similarities.
Building their songs
Existing research points to one main conclusion: Birdsong is
structured like human music. Songbirds change their tempo (speed), pitch (how
high or low they sing), and timbre (tone) to sing tunes that have specific
rules and resemble our own melodies.
Other features, like cadence and tension, are also used in
both birdsong and human music, said Tina Roeske, a behavioral neurobiologist
who specializes in birdsong.
Scientists are
finding more evidence that birdsong parallels human-made music.
Just as the familiar tune “In the Hall of the Mountain King”
gradually builds speed “accelerando,” as the compositional notation is known,
some birdsong does too, like that of the nightingale.
While earlier studies focused on syntax, or how notes were
ordered, newer research is integrating rhythm, too, by analyzing how notes are
timed. In human music, rhythm is often thought of as a constant beat, like the
one that opens “We Will Rock You” by Queen. But in birdsong, rhythm refers to
patterns of notes, regardless of whether they are repeated.
To humans, birdsong may appear to have “a random structure,”
Roeske said. Because of the speed at which birds sing — up to four times as
fast as most human music — that rhythm is “hard for us to grasp and
appreciate,” she added.
Roeske and her co-author, Tchernichovski, researched birds’
musical structure and found that birdsong rhythms fell into three general
categories: isochronous, in which intervals between notes are equidistant;
alternating, in which a note is longer than the previous one; and ornament, an
exaggerated form of the alternating pattern. Human music contains these
rhythmic patterns, too.
Birdsong rhythm
Other researchers are gaining insights by focusing on
birdsong rhythm.
“We found that rhythm and syntax have a relationship that
nobody has really thought about before,” said Jeffrey Xing, a graduate student
in psychology at the University of California, San Diego, and an author of a
September 2022 paper analyzing the song structure of the Australian pied
butcherbird.
Pied butcherbirds “seem to prefer some song rhythms over
others,” such as isochronous rhythm, Xing said. In some ways, these rhythmic
patterns follow rules like forms of poetry that have strict meter. A good
example is a sonnet.
“It’s a very rigid rhythmic structure that you have to
follow, and somehow the syntax of the words you use has to conform to that,” he
said.
Human brains and avian brains
Hollis Taylor has dedicated her life’s work as a violinist
and ornithologist to the pied butcherbird, a species she deems a fellow
musician.
Taylor, who analyzed the bird’s rhythmic structures with
Xing, records the birds’ songs in Australian deserts and savannas in the middle
of the night. Then, she transcribes their notes into musical notation.
“The musician in me recognizes the musician in them,” Taylor
said.
She has observed what appear to be warmup sessions,
rehearsals and singing contests. Other than humans, there’s only a “small club”
of species with an observed capacity to learn songs and vocal patterns, Taylor
said, including songbirds, parrots, hummingbirds, bats, elephants and some
marine mammals.
Neuroscience research points to the idea that this affinity
between birds and humans is not so unusual. In terms of musical ability, we are
more like birds than we are like our primate cousins or other mammals, said
Johan Bolhuis, a zoologist who specializes in the cognitive neurobiology of
birds and humans.
Our brains and songbirds’ brains have a similar way of
learning musicality. But the brains of monkeys and non-songbirds, like gulls,
are organized in a different way, Bolhuis said. It could be a sign of shared
creative abilities: Like humans, some songbird species seem to improvise based
on the song patterns they have learned.
But there’s no evidence that their songs have meaning,
Bolhuis said.
“In the mind of the great composers, they actually meant
something” with music, he said. “It’s not so much the case in birdsong.”
Also, birds have a limited repertoire, whereas with only a
limited number of items, the human mind “can be infinitely creative,” Bolhuis
said.
Researchers agree, however, that birdsong can communicate
identity. “They can recognize individuals just the way you and I can recognize
each other by our voices,” said Mike Webster, director of the Macaulay Library
at the Cornell Lab of Ornithology.
When birds from a certain area hear a familiar bird singing,
he explained, it’s no big deal. But if the same bird moves to a new area, the
birds there “go bananas” in a territorial uproar. In this sense, singing is
like a way for birds to identify themselves — but there may be more to it than
that.
Why do birds sing?
While scientists have studied birdsong for decades, they
know little about why and how birds select specific tunes and what counts as deliberate
communication versus meaningless song.
Through brain-imaging studies, neuroscientists have found
that the human brain responds to music most strongly along a particular neural
circuit that is activated when a person listens to a song perceived as
pleasant. Studies have shown that birdsong elicits the same response in female
birds, possibly as an evolutionary mechanism for mate attraction. But
scientists still wonder whether birds sing for entertainment in addition to
mating.
There are other mysteries. Ornithologists have observed
“bird chatter” in parrots, when two birds appear to be whispering to each
other. There are also nonvocal sounds, Webster said: Some birds snap their
wings, some drum on trees and others rub their feathers together as if playing
the violin. The purpose of these sounds — whether communicative, musical or
both — sits on the next frontier of ornithology research.
“We’ve just scratched the surface,” Webster said. “Birds are
constantly making sound, and I think most of the time we don’t really know why,
and we don’t really know what they’re saying to each other.”
Read more Music
Jordan News